Open Access | Research Article
This work is licensed under a Creative
Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
Message Convergence as Reassurance about the Safety of the Food Supply Concerning African Swine Fever Virus
* Corresponding author: Timothy L. Sellnow
Mailing address: Nicholson School of Communication and Media, University of Central Florida, 12405 Aquarius Agora Dr.,
Orlando, FL 32816-1344, USA.
Email: Timothy.Sellnow@ucf.edu
Received: 23 September 2022 / Revised: 10 January 2023 / Accepted: 10 Feburary 2023 / Published: 15 March 2023
DOI: 10.31491/AHTR.2023.02.002
Abstract
Foodborne illness remains a serious risk for consumers. Knowing their vulnerability to this risk makes audiences attentive to risk messages about food safety. Though much is known about risk and crisis communication responding to known cases of foodborne illnesses, less in known about reassuring messages that the food supply is safe. This study expands our understanding of message convergence by exploring how audiences react to convergent messages intended to reassure them that no risk exists. Focus groups observed and responded to a series of messages explaining that African swine fever, though a threat to the pork industry, is not a danger to consumers. Most focus group participants recognized and saw value in message convergence based on accurate science attributed to credible sources. The study concluded that message convergence is effective in promoting reassurance; however, message convergence cannot be fully effective or sustain its positive influence unless it is ethical, adapted based on continuous dialogue with and feedback from audiences, and maintained over time. Despite these rigorous demands, message convergence has clear potential as a communication strategy for providing reassurance to audiences about the safety of the food supply.
Keywords
Message convergence, foodborne illness, African swine fever, focus groups
Introduction
According to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, each year 48 million people, or 1 out of every 6
Americans, will contract a foodborne illness [1]. Given
the likelihood of being personally impacted by foodborne
pathogens, consumers are aware of some level of risk and
seek out the latest recalls, warnings, and information as
data becomes available [2]. Foodborne illnesses are widereaching and impact all areas of the agricultural, food production, and food service sectors [3].
The U.S. swine industry has faced several potentials and
realized crisis situations over the last decade, including
the Porcine epidemic diarrhea virus (PEDv) in 2013 and
more recently the COVID-19 pandemic. Though COVID-19 did not infect swine, the shortage of processing
and packing workers available to move pigs throughout
the food production process heavily impacted the industry, causing a backup of swine and resulting in the killing
(culling) of thousands of pigs in the U.S. The story was
reported by major broadcast, print, and social media outlets, and brought the spotlight back to the swine industry
[4]. The news coverage caused public outrage, and speculation began about the millions of healthy swine that were
euthanized because of poor planning. In addition, consumers were given an inside look at the pork production process, which created a new awareness of the potential risks
within the swine industry to both animals and humans.
One area of ongoing potential threat to the swine industry
is African swine fever virus (ASFv). ASFv is a highly
contagious and deadly virus that infects both feral and
farm-raised pigs. Fortunately, ASFv has never been found
within the U.S., but it has caused significant swine losses
around the world, including sub-Saharan Africa, China,
Mongolia, Vietnam, as well as within parts of the European Union [5]. Though ASFv is a significant potential
threat to the U.S. swine industry, the illness cannot be
transmitted to humans through human to animal contact,
and people will not become ill if they consume pork
products containing the virus [6]. Despite these biological
facts, consumers and other stakeholders in the swine industry are frequently forced to, as Trotta et al. [7] explain,
“unravel fake news” spread in the domain of veterinary
medicine generally and on the topic of ASFv specifically.
When confronted with a crisis, the stakeholders on many
levels seek to resolve uncertainty by locating information about “whether the crisis will affect them, how they
should think, and what they should do” [8]. As risk and
crisis communication research has progressed, the influence of consistent or convergent content shared by multiple sources in warning messages about specific agricultural threats has been consistently established [9]. This
research has focused on multiple government and media
sources identifying points of consistency in what is known
even when a great deal of uncertainty remains about the
risk or crisis at hand [2, 9, 10]. Less is known, however,
about the role of such message convergence from multiple
sources intended to reassure audiences about the absence
of risk. Specifically, how do message recipients, particularly consumers, respond to convergent messages focused
on reassuring them the food supply is safe?
This study expands our understanding of message convergence by exploring how audiences react to convergent
messages intended to reassure them that no risk exists.
Previous research has focused on message convergence
during high risk or crisis events. Specifically, a diverse audience of consumers viewed broadcast messages reassuring them that the swine industry is committed to a) avoiding the introduction of ASFv in the U.S. and b) that even
if ASFv were present in the U.S., the disease cannot infect
humans. Thus, this study seeks to answer the research
question: To what extent does message convergence reassure consumers about the safety of an animal food product
that is threatened by a novel disease? This study has the
potential to inform the risk communication process in the
agriculture industry by extending research of message design from the reactive to the proactive realm. Such proactive communication may prove beneficial for addressing
the ongoing threat of misinformation and disinformation
that Trotta et al. [7] identify among such diseases as
ASFv. We begin with a review of the message convergence literature, particularly the message convergence
framework. We then explain our materials and methods
used in the study, discuss our results, and provide conclusions and suggestions for future research.
Message Convergence Framework
The Message Convergence Framework (MCF) was born
out of Perelman and Olbrechts-Tyteca’s [11] treatise, The
New Rhetoric: A Treatise on Argumentation. The New
Rhetoric seeks to understand the processes by which
publics informally reason their way to conclusions and
resulting actions based on the multiple messages they receive [12]. The MCF explains that audiences of different
backgrounds, experiences, and values often share similar
reasoning for choosing a particular action or behavior.
Though past research has shown that identifying areas
where messages in risk-based scenarios converge can
help practitioners craft more effective messaging, little is
known about how the public will respond to risk-based
messaging that reassures audiences that the risk will not
impact them directly.
Anthony et al. [2] distilled three central propositions
for MCF based on the work of Perelman and OlbrechtsTyteca [11]. First, they noted that converging claims made
by distinct sources increases the perceived strength of the
claims. Second, they observed that the strength of convergence was influenced favorably by the importance of
the topic to the audience. Finally, they established that,
though convergence may be widely recognized by an audience, convergence can weaken or dissolve over time as
conflicting information is discovered and presented. Sellnow et al. [9] extended the third proposition to argue that
organizations challenged by convergent claims based on
false evidence, such as the fake news referred to by Trotta
et al. [7], can begin the convergence process anew by contesting those false claims.
Considerable work has been done to understand the role
of message convergence during crisis circumstances. For
example, Sellnow et al. [9] used the MCF to describe how
members of the pork industry worked with agriculture
journalists to present a convergent message during the
PEDv epidemic. They explain that the initial challenge
was to recognize the source of the mounting animal deaths
was indeed PEDv. Swine veterinarians were essential to
this process. Similarly, Zhao et al. [13] applied the MCF
to observe the role of trust in government agencies during
the COVID-19 pandemic. They documented patterns of
information seeking that demonstrate the importance of
trust in government sources in building positive message
convergence.
Distrust of government sources often arises through misinformation and false claims, or disinformation spread
through social media [14]. Social media is a primary
means through which false claims are spread rapidly and
broadly. Ye and Ki [15] explored the potential for organizations to cultivate message convergence on social media
when confronted by a crisis. Previous studies of organizational crisis communication within social media mainly
focused on communication within or between organizations. Less is known about the impact of multiple messages from multiple sources through social media on how
the public perceives the reputation of an organization.
Using the MCF, Ye and Ki [15] explored the impact of
crisis communication strategies and message convergence
on how audiences view an organization’s reputation on
Facebook in a preventable crisis. The results suggest that
organizational reputation was impacted by the consistency
between the organization’s Facebook posts and followers’
comments and was also influenced by the perceived credibility of those leaving comments. This study suggests
there is value in promoting positive message convergence
with the public—in this case, social media followers before, during, and after crises. The current project extends
the work of Ye and Ki [15] to explore the impact of message convergence to proactively reassure against potentially fallacious claims or unwarranted fears.
Incentive for seeking information is another factor in the
influence of message convergence. Explicitly, the motivation for seeking out information about prevailing issues,
particularly those requiring a scientific interpretation, varies according to the degree of importance audiences assign
to a topic [16]. Whether reporting of scientific information
on a risk topic was convergent or divergent was not influential unless the issue was perceived as important. This
finding offers further support for the observation of Anthony et al. [2] in their third proposition, that the impact
of convergence is influenced by the perceived importance.
Based on this evidence, the potential for convergent messages to influence audience perceptions is largely dependent on the degree to which they recognize or are inspired
to recognize the issue as important to them.
Although most research applying the MCF has focused
on crisis communication, a study by Herovic et al. [17]
did focus on reassurance prior to a crisis. Herovic and
colleagues conducted a case study using the MCF analyzing risk communication surrounding the L’Aquila earthquake. Several earth scientists were imprisoned after their
message prior to the deadly earthquake in L’Aquila was
interpreted as overly reassuring. The scientists were later
acquitted when further evidence was presented establishing that the convergent message was based largely on a
misinterpretation provided by a government spokesperson
who was not a scientist. The authors conclude that convergence, particularly in scientific messages, should not
exceed the evidence available.
In summary, message convergence has been studied frequently in crisis communication settings. The MCF, however, has been applied sparingly in the study of reassuring
messages presented prior to a crisis. This study seeks to
understand the function of reassuring scientific messages
shared proactively about the lingering threat of ASFv.
Methods
A qualitative approach was taken, using focus groups to collect and observe audience critiques of convergent messages intended to reassure them that ASFv was not a threat to humans or to the U.S. food supply. After viewing each of three videos, participants were asked to share their level of confidence that the pork supply would be safe to eat if ASFv infects farms.
Participants
A total of 39 people participated in the focus groups from a pool of 148 people contacted. A total of 44 people were initially contacted through email by six of the researchers working on the project through their personal and professional connections. The remaining 104 people were recruited through snowball sampling by asking the original 44 potential participants if they knew of anyone else who may be interested in participating in the study. Participants had to be U.S. residents, over the age of 18, consumers of pork products, or a person who prepared and served pork at home. Of the 39 participants, 19 self-identified as female and 20 as male. Participants self-identified their age as follows: (3) 18-24, (11) 25-34, (11) 35-44, (4) 45-54, (4) 55-64, (5) 65-74, (1) 75-84.
Procedure
Six focus groups were conducted between March 29,
2022, and March 31, 2022. Focus groups lasted 41-52
minutes with an average of 47 minutes. The time it took
to play the news video clips was not included in the total time. Informed consent was obtained for each focus
group participant. The focus groups were conducted and
recorded via Zoom technology. Focus group content was
transcribed by a professional transcription service between
March 31, 2022, and April 1, 2022. A total of 84 singlespaced pages was generated by the focus group discussions. Transcription did not include audio transcription of
the video clips played during each of the focus groups.
Focus group facilitators were trained in best practices for
the use of Zoom as a communication channel for focus
groups. Participants were asked to remain unmuted unless they had excessive background noise. All participants
were required to have their camera on. Participants were
encouraged to use the hand-raising emoji if they wanted
to alert the facilitator that they wanted to speak next while
someone was speaking; otherwise, participants were free
to talk at will. All facilitators reported there were no issues with interrupting, and that all participants actively
participated in the focus group with no reported issues.
Each focus group included three short videos. The videos were modeled after a broadcast news story lasting
between one and two minutes. Ecological validity was
established through consultation with a broadcast journalist who was hired to create the videos. Factual content for
the videos was taken directly from existing publications
and video material shared about ASFv by the swine industry and United States Department of Agriculture. The
first video included a single source reassuring audiences
they could not contract ASFv. The second video included
three sources with a convergent message that humans cannot contract ASFv. The third message included the three
sources and a reference to the pork industry as a whole
with an explanation of how biosecurity efforts are being
used by the swine industry to keep ASFv out of the U.S.
After each video, viewers were asked to discuss their level
of confidence that ASFv could not infect them. They were
then asked to describe their confidence in the source or
sources and to mention any information or sources they
felt were missing. The focus group followed the progression of showing a video, discussing the content, showing
the next video, discussing the content, showing the final
video, and discussing the content.
Analysis
The transcripts were analyzed qualitatively to identify key themes emerging from the participants. Initial questions for focus groups and the general coding categories were derived using an “etic analytical lens” established by “existing disciplinary knowledge” of MCF [18]. The coding categories began with knowledge of previous literature and allowed for discovery of nuance based on the messages’ intent to provide reassurance rather than warning. Thus, initial categories focused on the message’s presence or absence of convergence, perception of the sources presented, and the utility or perceived relevance of the message content. Coders read the transcripts independently using a constant comparison method to determine the viability of existing categories and the need for further combination, recategorization, or the establishment of new categories [19]. The coders then met face-to-face to share their coding subthemes and to identify areas where the themes could be combined or relabeled. Discussion continued until consensus was reached. Coders then selected representative examples from the transcripts for each subtheme. Table 1 shows the themes and subthemes included in the analysis.
Results
Responses were grouped thematically according to the participants’ perceptions of convergence in general, specific questions regarding the content of the messages, references specific to biosecurity, and other (Table 1). Representative quotations from participants are provided for each theme and subtheme. Respondents whose quotations appear in the results are coded by two numbers. Each focus group and each individual within each focus group were given a number. For example, person one in focus group two is referred to as participant #2-1. We begin with a discussion of participants’ awareness of the absence, presence, and value of convergence.
Table 1
Themes and subthemes identified in focus group transcripts.
Themes | Subthemes |
---|---|
Recognition of Convergence | |
Absence of Convergence Presence of Convergence Value of Convergence |
|
Critiquing Message Content | |
Effective Message Content Message Content Weaknesses Missing Information Credibility of Message Sources |
|
Biosecurity | |
Fear-Inducing Reassuring |
|
Other | |
Imagery Financial Concerns |
Recognition of Convergence
As mentioned above, focus group participants viewed three messages differing in the number of sources providing convergent content. The third video also mentioned biosecurity as a form of convergence with other messages. Participants noted both an absence of convergence in the first video and the presence of convergence in the second and third videos.
(1) Absence of Convergence
After viewing the first message with a single source, multiple participants recognized the lack of convergence in
the first video. Participant #6-6 noted the problems with
hearing from just one voice, suggesting: Yeah, I also feel
like just watching that video and getting all of our information from one person and nothing else, like information
can be… you can get it from one person anywhere and it
can be completely wrong. So, who are we to just trust this
one person that we’ve never seen before. I’ve never been
to Iowa, so I don’t know who she is.
Similarly, Participant #4-6 pointed out that, in the first
video, “It seemed like they didn’t give nearly enough information. They gave like one source from one random
veterinarian. If they would have given more sources overall, I feel like it would have been more useful.” Participant
#6-3 shared a similar perspective, noting: I feel like the
fact that they only gave one side and one person talking
and then they just cut it back, it’s not… I mean I think you
need more sources to make something reliable or something at least a little bit believable. So just having her talk
and then that’s it, I don’t think it’s as credible as having
multiple sources.
Clearly, the lack of convergence was a recognized weakness in the first message.
(2) Presence of Convergence
In addition to recognizing the lack of convergence in the first video, many participants recognized the presence of convergence in the subsequent videos. Comparing the second video to the first video, Participant #3-5 commented, “I think it was nice that the second video incorporated those other sources. I think it makes the message stronger.” Likewise, Participant #3-1 valued having more than one source confirming the information, suggesting that “having somebody who is a secondary expert, or, like, just as, just a second expert at all, I think, it, kind of, helps out a bit.” Participant #3-4 agreed, stating, “I think having more people speak made me feel more confident.” Reflecting on the alignment of the sources, Participant #2-2 noted, “Whenever for me you have multiple sources confirming the same trajectory or the same direction, it always feels a little more confidence producing.”
(3) Value of Convergence
In addition to identifying the presence and absence of convergence, some participants specifically reflected on the
value of convergence. Participant #2-4 explained: I just
think the fact that they all agreed 100% with each other,
there was not really any variation in their message. It was
safe, and it did not cross over to humans. Nobody had a
different message. That was confidence producing.
Along these lines, Participant #3-4 stated, “Ultimately, it
really wouldn’t matter to me who gave the message, it’s
more about the consistencies in the message.” Others saw
the benefit of having multiple voices from multiple areas
of expertise. As Participant #2-1 suggested, they preferred
the video(s) that demonstrated convergence because “each
one... brought just a little bit of something different to the
table. They brought a little bit more information or a little
bit more background from it.” The value of convergence
was also noted through its contrast with the first video’s
lack of convergence. As Participant #5-1 suggested: I
think when we watch one person who is acting as the expert message sender, that is a red flag to me. It’s like saying nine out of ten dentists surveyed that Crest is the best toothpaste, but did we survey a million dentists or only
ten? I don’t question her credibility in her field, but that
one person is not enough for me.
The value of convergence was clearly expressed by the
majority of study participants.
A few participants had an alternative perspective on the
value of convergence. For instance, there was also some
concern over the possible bias in sources; as Participant #
6-4 noted: The source was an Iowa veterinarian and they
made it pretty clear that Iowa is the number one pork supplier in this country. They all have a vested interest in continuing to keep our confidence in their economy basically.
So, I would want one more, at least one more, confirming
source to make sure that that wasn’t just a biased view.
Further, while the aim of the message was to reassure
participants that the pork they eat is safe, the following
example from Participant #4-4 illustrates where the convergence had the opposite effect. They stated, “I think I
do have less confidence in it now because you know, one
story is interesting. The second story kind of elaborated a
little more. And it’s like yeah, I think there is something
going on.”
Critiquing Message Content
Although most of the study participants saw convergence as a notable asset to the ASFv messages, a minority saw the convergence as problematic. An explanation of how respondents critiqued the content of the messages provides further insight into what they valued and what concerned them about the message content and convergence.
(1) Effective Message Content
Many participants were eager to share their views on the
value of the content being shared in the videos. One of
the categories of information most frequently cited as being helpful was information on how the disease cannot
be transmitted to humans. When asked if they would still
eat pork after viewing the videos, Participant #3-5 stated,
“Yeah, I would still eat it. I mean it said that it’s not a concern for humans. So that was key for me.” Similarly, Participant #2-4 expressed that when it came to eating pork,
they were “still confident. I think that they stressed in the
last video that this had been studied for a really long time.
The fact that it cannot jump to humans and that it was biologically cell-specific for pigs.” Participant #5-7 added:
Particularly, I’m more comfortable that the virus won’t
pass to a human. They made that very clear, and they talked about the people who have spent hours and hours and
hours around these pigs and have never gotten the virus.
Along these lines, some participants noted that they found
the scientific data shared to be particularly valuable. Participant #2-6 stated, “I feel like the data points in this were
a little bit more scientifically backed, and therefore easier
to believe.” Likewise, Participant #4-6 expressed: I’ll say
that I was a lot more convinced by this video because
the narrator also said that this was a scientific fact that it
wouldn’t spread to human. Like I would go out and do my
own research as well to make sure that was legitimate, but
it was also more reassuring to me, like oh, this is obviously scientific fact.
Participant #2-6 also touched on the value of sharing scientific data and the ability to verify it for oneself, noting
that, “These individuals provided the data for at least me
to be able to go look up more information on my own if
I chose.” This sentiment was also shared by Participant
#5-8, who expressed approval when they added, “you’d
certainly be able to at least research more sources.”
(2) Message Content Weaknesses
Some information was seen as being unhelpful. For example, Participant #5-7 perceived a contradiction: Unless
I missed something, they’re mostly focused on the virus
transmitting to a human, not transmitting to a human
through eating of the food. I don’t—unless I missed it—
nobody has said yet that the virus doesn’t live in cooked
pork, or you won’t have to worry about this even if the
pig you eat had the virus as long as your pork is fully
cooked. I still don’t feel comfortable, because I feel like
they’re focused on the wrong thing. Yeah, I’m not even
going to be around a pig, so getting the virus from a pig
doesn’t concern me. It’s more what if I eat the pork that
came from the pig, and I don’t feel like that’s been really
thoroughly addressed.
Several other participants perceived similar inconsistencies in messaging that they considered troubling. For
example, Participant #3-4 suggested: There were a couple
things that were a little, like, “I’m not sure about that.”
For instance, they said, on the one hand it doesn’t affect
humans at all; on the other hand, we’re working hard to
make sure none of the contaminated pork gets in the human food supply. And it’s, like, “Well, why do you care if
it’s in the human food supply if it doesn’t affect humans at
all?”
Participant #5-4 made a similar observation, stating, “To
me, it’s very contradicting in a particular point. It says it
does not affect the food chain at all, but yet at the same
time, it kills half of the supply. What?”
Another type of information participants expressed suspicion towards was when it appeared that speakers were
making claims without evidence. Participant #1-1 stated:
I honestly say that my confidence is actually a little lower
than it was after the first video. I did like the information provided about, you know, that the certain cell that is
unique to the pig which is why it can’t infect humans. But
I felt like there was more emphasis on USDA not letting
“bad” meat into the system.
Similarly, Participant #2-6 asked, “Has there been a study
where people have eaten that particular meat and did not
come down with it?”.
Interestingly, several participants felt that the information
and warnings being provided made the issue seem like it
was actually more serious than was being publicly shared.
As Participant #1-2 asked, “I’m like why would they be
on alert if nowhere in America or you know, everything is
so safe.” Participant #3-4 had a similar view, warning: I
think you have to be careful, though, about adding some
about food safety because for me, that could almost have
the opposite effect. Because if you say, “Here’s how you
need to cook your meat,” then I’d be, like, you’re lying to
me. This is really going to affect humans and that’s why you’re telling me.
Though respondents found value in much of the message
content, some participants also had concerns about elements of message content. In addition, some participants
found the messages failed to provide key information they
wanted to know.
(3) Missing Information
The participants were particularly vocal about the information they felt was missing from the videos. One of the
reoccurring items they pointed out was they wanted to
hear about the processes in place to ensure sick animals
do not enter the food chain. For example, Participant #
1-1 wanted to see more about the testing protocols or
procedures: Like how are they verifying that it isn’t here
yet after making those claims. And what are they going to
do if it is detected? But it, the second half of that isn’t as
important to me as the first half of that. Well, how are you
ensuring that it isn’t here?
Similarly, Participant #1-3 expressed a desire to see
“Maybe how they test and what percentage of hogs are
tested. Do they do a sampling of five percent or 50 percent
or more? That would be nice to know.” Along these lines,
Participant #6-2 suggested “Maybe also include testing
procedures or what they’re doing for testing. Like how are
they identifying the sick pigs other than the pigs lying on
their sides that we saw in the video.”
Another question was, as Participant #4-7 inquired, “I
would like to know like are they working on developing
like a vaccine for these hogs? Are they, you know, are
they working on some sort of medication they can take?”
Additionally, some participants wanted to know if there
are ways of checking if the meat is contaminated. Participant #4-7 asked: Maybe the industry would layer on some
sort of testing to assure you that like meat is tested for and
for being virus free. And then, you know, so you had that
assurance when you were buying it like oh, this is antibiotic free and you know, swine flu, swine fever free.
Similarly, some participants wanted to hear more about
food preparation safety measures they could take to reduce the risk of contamination. As Participant #1-3 commented: I was just curious as to whether there has been
any testing to see if a contaminated animal gets through
the system, does cooking it, making it or any food preparation that they recommended temperature for pork eliminate the contamination or does it get through the cooking
process also.
Likewise, Participant #3-3 noted, “I don’t know if you
have to cook at more temperature or less, what? Maybe
a little piece from a food expert would’ve, instead of just
pork.”
Multiple participants were curious where the disease has
spread so far. Some were particularly concerned whether
it may have unknowingly spread to the U.S. Participant
#2-6 asked: When they are sitting there saying it is not
here, where are the data points to support that? Did they
check three pigs and say it is not here? Have they tested
every pig to say it is not here? Is it only Indiana, but maybe it exists in California?
Another area participants felt the videos lacked information on was how other countries are dealing with the issue. As Participant #5-6 expressed: If we had heard maybe
something about what farmers and the agriculture industry
is doing in Asia where the problem is much more severe,
or I guess existing at all. Hearing what they’re doing,
that’s good. You know, even though a lot of their pigs are
sick, no one over there is getting sick from eating pork.
So, something they’re doing, seeing how they’re processing their livestock in terms of the biosecurity that you
were mentioning would give a little more confidence.
Relatedly, some participants also wanted to learn about
the case numbers so far. As Participant #2-3 noted, “I
would like to know the number of cases in countries that
we import pork from.”
Some participants wanted to learn more about practical
health impacts, such as how the disease is transmitted and
how it might impact humans and the meat they eat. As
Participant #1-1 suggested: They said that it won’t affect
humans. But I don’t think they said in what way. So, you
mean like the infectious disease itself cannot be transmitted to us through certain touch or however, or is that
through eating of the meat, if that animal did have this
disease and it went through the whole process kind of and
then like what was said like cooking a certain temperature take that risk away. But it just said it couldn’t affect
humans but not necessarily in what way, at least I don’t
think they did.
Extending this observation, Participant #1-6 questioned,
“What are the side effects of consuming contaminated
meat?”
Some participants had hoped to glean more statistical and
graphical representation of data in the messages. Participant #4-6 commented: I was going to add as well, in all
three videos there was a lack of statistical data. I feel like
if they would have shown like a bar graph of how the
disease has progressed over time, sort of like how they’ve
done with COVID.
Several other participants expressed the desire to see more
graphics, charts, or visual aids adding to the information
being presented in the videos. This included scientific visuals, as reflected in.
Participant #6-1’s comment, “I would like to see graphs
and charts that are tracking everything. And just use
comparative data between the different countries and see
where the exact red zones are and see if there is a spike in
sicknesses in people.”
(4) Credibility of Message Sources
In addition to the content presented in the videos, the
sources sharing the information were subject to both
praise and criticism by the participants. Multiple participants commented on the qualifications of the individuals
in the videos. To that end, some participants questioned
the relevancy and ethos of the state veterinarian solely
featured in the first video. As Participant #2-6 noted, “If
the person that takes care of my cats says the food is safe
to eat, I don’t know if that is the person I want to be relying to for truth to source on that.” This elicited the opposite reaction from at least one participant, Participant
#3-6, who also commented on the state veterinarian: But I mean, I trusted the veterinarian because, I mean, they
weren’t a pig farmer, and they weren’t somebody directly
that, like, benefits. I mean, like, they treat the animal, but
they don’t directly benefit from profits. So, I was just, I
trusted them.
A second veterinarian/professor who was shown working
with pigs on a farm was on the whole viewed as being
more qualified than the state veterinarian who spoke from
a desk and a public health expert. For example, Participant
#3-3 stated: I thought the second veterinarian, it added
some, almost accountability because I think they said that
he also writes the rules for the FDA. So, if he is out there
saying, like, it’s not the threat. So yeah, it adds credibility,
and a little bit of accountability, I think.
Beyond their individual qualifications, many participants commented on the agencies these individuals represent. Several participants expressed skepticism over
government agencies and those who belong to them,
particularly the FDA. Participant #1-2 stated: I believe
our government is interested in safe food and United
States compared to other countries, especially third world
countries, etc., you know, is known for their safety. But I
am skeptical of, you know, FDA and those knowing what
they approve and what they don’t approve in other areas
outside of like the pork.
Although the sources featured in the videos were perceived as credible by the majority of participants, a few
participants raised minor concerns about the motives of
the sources and the agencies they represent.
Biosecurity
When commenting on the biosecurity elements included in the third video, the study participants were divided in their reaction. Some felt the added reference attributed to the pork industry about biosecurity heightened their fear, while others found the message reassuring. Still others questioned whether the added mention of biosecurity was intended to manipulate their reaction to ASFv.
(1) Fear Inducing
Some participants felt that references to biosecurity were
fear-inducing, despite the intention of these references to
reassure people that the appropriate safety measures were
being taken. As Participant #5-3 commented, “That whole
thing about the bio thing, it was kind of like introducing
a nuclear wasteland or something to it, so not giving me
much confidence.”
It seems that the visuals associated with biosecurity measures heightened participants’ concern and sense that
something was wrong. Participant #6-4 elaborated: I personally I think probably went backwards in the wrong direction after that video. Just some of the visual. Like why
do the humans have to sanitize themselves in a shower in
between entering a facility if there’s no danger of cross
contamination between humans and pigs? And just more
showing how many precautions they’re taking, like in between herds being brought into the food processing plants.
It just seems like that almost frightened me more.
Comments such as these suggest that some participants
recognized their impressions were counter to the intent of
the video. As Participant #5-8 astutely noted: It definitely
seemed to be doing more…well, it was putting more time
into explaining what was being done to keep infected animals from ending up in our food, but I can see how some
of the imagery might spur more concern over it. So, I can
see that it was trying to spend more time allaying those
fears, but ironically, some people are seeing that as more
concerning.
In short, a minority of participants actually felt less reassured after viewing the biosecurity content.
(2) Reassuring
While the biosecurity measures heightened some people’s
fear and concern, other participants viewed them in a
more positive light. Participant #3-1 noted, “I felt a lot
more confidence with the ending section, specifically
where they were showing imagery of actually spraying
down, like, the hog pens.” These biosecurity measures
were viewed by the majority of participants as being helpful and consistent with their understanding, as Participant
#4-7 commented: I felt a little bit more confident seeing
the procedures that are taken to help or to help prevent
this from spreading to you know, American farms. And it
seemed to make sense. Like it was logical and consistent
with what I would expect might be in place to help prevent that.
Participant #3-6 pointed out that the positive association
with cleaning measures may be related to the recent COVID-19 pandemic, suggesting: I think the images of the
pens being cleaned was more reassuring than it was, like,
confidence boosting. But I think that’s also just because
we’re coming off of, like, the corona, coronavirus, and,
like, everybody had to clean everything, and just, like,
general cleanliness needs.
Other Findings
While most of discussion fit within themes related to message convergence, two extraneous sub-categories were also uncovered: video imagery and financial concerns.
(1) Imagery
Some respondents made note of the imagery in the videos.
For instance, with regard to the video imagery, Participant #1-4 observed: The images that were used were like
concerning to me because I thought oh, no, I thought pigs
were treated better in places. And the way that they were
just all squished together, it made me think oh, if it comes
here, it’s definitely like, it’s going to spread because those
don’t look like sanitary conditions. So, the imagery used
was honestly, it grossed me out.
In contrast, other participants expressed an appreciation
for the video imagery and production quality, noting that
it added to the credibility of the message for them. Specifically, Participant #3-6 said (regarding the impact of the
video imagery on credibility) “it’s a little bit of both, it’s,
like, the fact that there were more sources, and the fact
that the production crew put more effort into it”.
(2) Financial Concerns
Financial concerns were also observed as a sub-category
that arose in multiple focus groups. For instance, Participant #4-2 expressed: I also wondered just how it will affect, potentially affect pork prices. You know, if you have
to kill off a bunch of hogs and you’re only leaving a few
good ones, like it’s going to, I would assume, raise the
price a lot. So, I guess that would be some sort of impact
it would have on the market.
Similarly, Participant #2-5 stated: I think the other concern is the price of pork could possibly go up if they are
slaughtering all these pigs that are contaminating, and
they could spread it. We could be running out of pork in
the supermarkets again like we did with chickens a couple
of years ago.
These financial concerns seemed to be shared by a number of a participants, as Participant #2-4 also noted, “I was
more concerned about the $15 pound of bacon I was buying.” For some, this concern seemed to be one of the more
salient themes of all three videos, leading Participant #1-6
to question the intent of the videos altogether: I’m more
questioning who the actual target audience is. I know that
there is a swine [problem], right, like making sure you are
not eating contaminated meat, but really is this preparing
us for an increase in price and a disruption in distribution,
and impact to the farmer, or is this really a health alert?
Though discussion of imagery and financial concerns
were consistent, they occupied very little of the focus
group discussion time.
Discussion
This study explored the potential for message convergence
to reassure audiences about an important issue before
it potentially manifested into a high risk or crisis event.
ASFv is a looming threat to the U.S. food supply but is
not currently present in the country. The messages progressing from an absence of convergence to convergence
and ending with convergence and biosecurity examples
did, for most study participants, provide reassurance that
the food supply is and would remain safe. Thus, MCF is
viable for fostering reassurance in pre-crisis contexts. This
overall finding, however, comes with several cautions regarding message content in general and messages emphasizing biosecurity specifically.
Given the ability of participants to distinguish between
the presence and absence of message convergence and the
majority of participants’ perception of the value of convergence, the strategy of providing convergence in messages
early in discussions of evolving risk issues is advisable.
Trotta et al. [7] commiserate with veterinarians who must
toil with sources of what they label fake news as they seek
to communicate the facts of ASFv and other animal diseases. Providing explanations of ASFv and other diseases
with clear representations of message convergence is one
means of adding positive influence to risk discussions.
Doing so provides an early countermeasure to the misinformation that can spread quickly about risk issues. This
study suggests, however, that message convergence can
benefit from ongoing public dialogue and feedback.
The majority of study participants recognized and saw
value in message convergence. A minority of participants,
however, also identified content they felt was missing
and concerns about some of the sources providing convergent messages. For example, some participants were
not persuaded by the convergent messages because they
remained fearful that they had not heard enough reassurance about how to cook potentially contaminated pork.
Adding this information to reassuring messages is not difficult; however, practitioners would not necessarily know
cooking procedures are important to an audience without
engaging them in dialogue or seeking feedback to their
initial messages. Similarly, a minority of participants had
concerns about individual sources and the agencies they
represent. Although the concerns were not consistent toward any single source or agency, the fact remains that individuals view convergent messages with preconceptions
about some sources. Dialogue and feedback are needed to
recognize when a particular source or agency is not seen
as credible for a particular audience or subgroup. Thus,
future applications of MCF in research and consultation
with practitioners should include an emphasis on dialogue
and feedback to determine what aspects of message convergence are problematic. Without corrective action for
problems perceived by audiences, message convergence
cannot reach its positive potential.
Perelman and Olbrechts-Tyteca [11] introduced message
convergence with the expectation that audiences would
continuously seek information about topics of importance
to them. In response, the propositions Anthony et al.
created emphasize that convergence is ongoing and can
change over time [2]. Indeed, participants in this study
were complimentary of the fact the information shared in
the videos could serve as a basis for their future inquiries.
In contrast, some participants indicated that they simply
needed more information and intended to find it on their
own. Neither response is surprising given the findings
of previous studies [9, 20]. The point is clear. A single
message providing convergence is a step forward, not a
destination. Audiences will continuously seek or demand
strategy information about issues of importance. Thus,
message convergence is a form of risk communication
that must be undertaken with regulatory on given risk issues if it is to have the level of influence advocated by
Trotta et al. [7].
This study also revealed that a minority of participants
feared the message convergence they observed could have
been contrived or manipulated. Such doubts in the veracity
of claims can thwart efforts to inspire reassurance. Again,
feedback and dialogue are needed to assess the degree to
which particular audiences perceive such manipulation.
Once known, practitioners can and should respond with
accurate and ethical information audiences want or need
to hear. Herovic et al. [17], however, offer a stern warning
against attempts to over-reassure an audience by making
claims that outpace the scientific evidence available. If the
evidence available at the time is not convincing for audiences, despite high levels of convergence, practitioners
should show patience. Herovic et al. [17] document the
harrowing results of over-reassurance and communicating
beyond scientific fact in an earthquake-prone area.
Biosecurity was introduced in the third message as an
example of convergence on best practices for controlling the spread of ASFv. Although the majority of study
participants found the biosecurity messages reassuring,
a minority responded that the protective measures were
ironically stress-inducing. Seeing brief footage of workers
preparing to shower in and shower out of a swine facility
and watching individuals spraying facilities with pressure
washers made some feel the situation was much worse
than they were actually being told. Why, they wondered,
would such measures be needed if ASFv is not in the U.S.
Future efforts to study the potential influence of biosecurity measures to promote convergence need to include a
more general explanation explaining that biosecurity is
an ongoing practice used to protect animals from multiple
disease threats, not a measure introduced occasionally
when severe risks or crises are imminent.
Summary
Foodborne illness remains a serious risk for consumers. Knowing their vulnerability to this risk makes audiences attentive to risk messages about food safety. These messages, however, are as prone to misinformation as any other risk issue [7]. Message convergence has the potential to reassure audiences through clearly explained scientific evidence that is endorsed by multiple, credible sources. This risk communication strategy, however, cannot be fully effective or sustain its positive influence unless message convergence is provided ethically, adapted based on continuous dialogue with and feedback from audiences, and maintained over time. Despite these rigorous demands, message convergence has clear potential as a communication strategy for providing reassurance to audiences well before an issue reaches high risk or crisis levels.
Declarations
Authors’ contributions
Performed data analysis and interpretation as well as data acquisition and administrative support: Mayer M, Vega J. Performed data acquisition as well as provided administrative and analytical support: Demarco I, Eicher R, Flores Molina D, Lee C, Perez Naufel J, Sheng X, Sublette J. Made substantial contributions to study planning and execution and reviewed the literature: Freihaut R. Made substantial contributions to conception and design of the study and performed data analysis and interpretation: Sellnow TL.
Availability of data and materials
Data supporting our findings can be retrieved from the corresponding author upon request.
Financial support and sponsorship
This research was supported, in part, by a grant from the United States Department of Agriculture; National Institute of Food and Agriculture [2015-69004-23273].
Conflicts of interest
All authors declared that there are no conflicts of interest.
Ethical approval and informed consent statement
The University of Central Florida Institutional Review Board approved this study (Ref #STUDY00003893). The approved informed consent form is pictured below.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
References
1.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Estimates of foodborne illness in the United States. [Cited 2018 Nov 5]. Available from:https://www.cdc.gov/foodborneburden/index.html
2. Anthony K E, Sellnow T L, & Millner A G. Message convergence as a message-centered approach to analyzing and improving risk communication. J Appl Commun Res, 2013, 41(4): 346-364. [Crossref]
3. Won S, Goodwin BK, & Boys KA. Empirical modeling of the risk and determinants associated with food-related illnesses. AgEconSearch, 2021. [Crossref]
4. Gauly M, Chemineau P, Rosati A, & Sartin J. COVID-19 pandemic-How and why animal production suffers? Anim Front, 2021, 11(1): 3-6. [Crossref]
5. U.S. Food & Drug Administration. African Swine Fever. [Cited 2022 Apr 18]. Available from: https://www.fda.gov/animal-veterinary/safety-health/african-swinefever
6. United States Department of Agriculture Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service. African Swine Fever. [Cited 2018 Nov 18]. Available from: https://www.aphis.usda.gov/publications/animal_health/asf.pdf
7. Trotta A, Marinaro M, Cavalli A, Cordisco M, Piperis A, Buonavoglia C, et al. African Swine Fever-How to unravel fake news in veterinary medicine. Animals, 2022, 12(5): 656. [Crossref]
8. Seeger MW, Sellnow TL, & Ulmer RR. Communication and organizational crisis: Greenwood Publishing Group, 2003.
9. Sellnow, DD, Sellnow TL, & Martin JM. Strategic message convergence in communicating biosecurity: The case of the 2013 porcine epidemic diarrhea virus. Commun Rep, 2019, 32(3): 125-136. [Crossref]
10. Sellnow DD, Lane DR, Sellnow TL, & Littlefield RS. The IDEA model as a best practice for effective instructional risk and crisis communication. Commun Stud, 2017, 68(5): 552-567. [Crossref]
11. Perelman C, Olbrechts-Tyteca L. The new rhetoric: A treatise on argumentation. London: University of Notre Dame Press, 1969.
12. McKerrow RE. The centrality of justification: Principles of warranted assertability. In: Williams DC, Hazen MD, editors. Argumentation theory and the rhetoric of assent. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press 1990.
13. Zhao X, Xu S, & Austin, LL. Medium and source convergence in crisis information acquisition: Patterns, antecedents, and outcomes. New Media Soc, 2022. [Crossref]
14. Eriksson M. Lessons for crisis communication on social media: A systematic review of what research tells the practice. Int J Strateg Commun, 2018, 12(5): 526-551. [Crossref]
15. Ye L, Ki E. Impact of message convergence on organizational reputation: An examination of organizational crisis communication on Facebook. Corp Reput Rev, 2018, 21: 1-8. [Crossref]
16. Jensen JD, Hurley RJ. Conflicting stories about public scientific controversies: Effects of news convergence and divergence on scientists’ credibility. Public Underst Sci, 2012, 21(6): 689-704. [Crossref]
17. Herovic E, Sellnow TL, & Anthony KE. Risk communication as interacting arguments: Viewing the L’Aquila earthquake disaster through the message convergence framework. Argum Advocacy, 2014, 51(2): 73-86. [Crossref]
18. Lindlof TR, Taylor BC. Qualitative communication research methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage, 2011.
19. Lincoln YS, Guba EG. Naturalist inquiry. Newbury Park: Sage 1985. 416 p.
20. Anthony KE, Sellnow TL. The role of the message convergence framework in medical decision-making. J Health Commun, 2016, 21: 249-256. [Crossref]